Teaching Synaesthesia as a
Gateway to Creativity
Monica D. Murgia
Abstract
This article encapsulates my
experience of teaching creativity within a higher education curriculum.
Creativity often eludes common understanding because it involves using
different conceptual streams of thought, often times developing unconsciously
and manifesting in the prized “eureka” moment. In 2009, I began explaining the neurological
condition of synaesthesia and later introduced this phenomenology in a course
designed to cultivate creativity to first year fashion design students. There are many challenges in
teaching creativity. Through teaching this course, I discovered that the first
challenge is making the students conscious of their own qualitative beliefs on
creativity and art. The second is creating exercises to challenge and alter
these beliefs, thus forming a new way of thinking and experiencing the world. The
most resistance from my students arose when experimenting with
non-representational art. They did not have a conscious framework for making
and evaluating abstract art. Introducing synaesthesia, a neurologically-based
condition that “merges” two or more sensory pathways in the brain, gave my
students a framework for discovery. Understanding sensory modalities and ways
in which these modalities can blended together in synaesthesia proved to be a
gateway to creativity in many of my students. The scope of this article
chronicles how I developed my teaching methodology, the results it created in
my classroom, as well as its effects on my own artistic practice.
Keywords: synaesthesia;
creativity; arts practice; teaching; cross-modal
Introduction
The
great enigma in fashion and art lies around creativity: Where does it come
from? Is it innate in some people, and
missing in others? Is it something that can be fostered, and if so, how? I am a
firm believer that creativity, however elusive and indefinable, is something
that can be taught. In fact, there are many things that have to be “unlearned”
to allow creativity to grow. The visual experience is whitewashed with
memories, thoughts, and ideas of what should be present, instead of what is
actually there.
In The
Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms, Margaret A. Boden shares a similar
view. She explains that creativity is rooted in everyday abilities such as
conceptual thinking, perception, and memory. Therefore, to some degree everyone
is creative. Boden explains that there are three different types of creativity:
the first involves making unfamiliar combination of familiar ideas; the second
is exploration of conceptual spaces; and the third is transformation of
conceptual spaces (Boden, 2004: 3-4). What makes some people more creative,
then, is their ability to explore and transform their own ideas and mental
processes.
In
2010, I began teaching a college course, entitled Fashion Seminar. This
course was for first year fashion design students, and aimed to help them
develop an identity as a designer and stimulate creativity within the design
discipline. A large component of the class involved developing an art
portfolio. The portfolio was comprised of exercises that encouraged the student
develop creative and abstract thinking skills. Each student was required to
select a natural and manmade image as inspiration. The goal of each weekly
portfolio assignment was to challenge the students to see these images in new
ways. By repeatedly looking at the same images, the portfolio exercises
challenged the students to take something familiar and examine it different
exercises, perspectives, and reinterpret it with various media. Each week, an
assignment was presented to challenge the students to adjust their visual
perceptions in order to encourage their creativity.
I
taught this course for a year and a half, with as many as 5 separate sections
of the same course in a term. Over a number of terms of teaching this course,
the most successful assignment, in terms of the creative output it generated, was
called “multiple sensory”. As the title suggests, the student has to take an
inspirational image and create a visual representation of a multi-sensory experience
evoked by that image. The learning outcome was aimed at encouraging the
students to experiment with nonrepresentational, or abstract, art. In abstract
art, the selection of media and colour combinations is more important than a
recognizable image. This type of art creates a visceral reaction within the
viewer. Many of my students had no previous experience creating abstract art,
so this exercise served as a gateway to experiment with it. To properly
communicate the idea of representing multi-sensory experience, I developed a
curriculum that included case studies from neuroscience. In particular, I
discussed the phenomenon of synaesthesia, a neurological condition, to help
students develop new methods of creating art. Introducing and describing synaesthsia
in the classroom gave the students a structure for experimenting with abstract
art. Over time, they formed new was for observing, creating, and evaluating
non-representational forms For example,
one student, ‘Monet B’, took the inspirational image of a woman smoking. ‘Monet
B’ then created a series of small paintings that expressed a visual
representation of the smell and taste of smoking.
Phenomenology of Synaesthesia
Synaesthesia is a neurologically-based condition in which stimulation of
one sensory pathway (e.g. visual pathway) leads to automatic, involuntary
experiences in a second sensory pathway (e.g. aural pathway). This means that two or more senses merge
together instead of remaining separate. For instance, some synaesthetes—those
that have synaesthesia— will see colours when they hear sound or touch objects.
Every
case of synaesthesia is different. Some people see colours while tasting food. Others
hear sounds from the smell of fragrances. Some can taste sounds and images. There
are more than 100 different types of synaesthesia. The most commonly reported
phenomenon is coloured hearing, that is, sounds producing a visual
manifestation. Seeing letters or numbers in colours is also widely reported. Each
letter or number has a corresponding colour.
The
early researchers of synaesthesia were Heinrich Kluver (1897-1979) and Georg
Anschutz (1886-1953), both of whom worked independently. Frustrated by
romanticized, poetic, and vague descriptions of what synaesthetes were
experiencing, they conducted rigorous studies with the collaboration of synaesthetes
to peer inside their minds, and produce a classification of the experience.
These studies included asking the synaesthetes to create works of art as a way
of capturing their subjective experience.
Synaesthesia
became a romantic ideal in literature, but was widely regarded as an imaginary
condition, or a drug-induced hallucination. However, the advent of modern
medicine affirmed that synaesthesia is a real neurological condition. Dr.
Vilayanur S. Ramachandran and Dr. Edward M. Hubbard have pioneered research and
education on synaesthesia, demonstrating through their clinical experiments
that synaesthetes experience a cross wiring of different sensory-processing
regions in the brain (Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2003: 54). In fact, they have
produced evidence of this cross wiring of the sensory pathways in the colourblind:
'We also observed one case in which we believe cross activation enables a colourblind
synaesthete to see numbers tinged with hues he otherwise cannot perceive; charmingly,
he refers to these as "Martian colours"' (Ramachandran and
Hubbard, 2003: 57).
It
is difficult to say how many people have synaesthesia. First of all, those that
experience this blending of the senses since birth do not see it as a
“condition”. This is their regular way of living. Therefore, synaesthetes do
not understand that other people have different sensory experiences. Secondly,
while research has been conducted on synaesthesia since the 1880s, findings
have not been widely distributed. It has been estimated that as many as 1 per
every 100 people experiences synaesthesia. After one of my workshops, a student
approached me and said that he experienced the exact phenomenon I explained. Sean
let me briefly interview him. The full recording is available here https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U54FYiTR2pI,
in which he explains:
“When I hear music it’s like an instant graphic. . . . I thought it
was normal. Music is a constant inspiration for me, because it’s like an
instant installation in my mind.”
To
a certain degree, everyone experiences synaesthesia; Stroop interference tests
are perfect illustrations (De Young, 2014). Stroop interference tests use the
words to describe a colour but are written in a different coloured ink. For
example, the word blue is printed in red ink. You are asked to identify the
word, and ignore the colour – which after a succession of coloured words
becomes tricky to instantaneously identify the word. The mind starts to
associate more quickly with the colour of the text, instead of the meaning of
the text. According to a scientific study, synaesthesia is seven times more
common in creative people than in the general population (Ramachandran and
Hubbard, 2003: 57).
Teaching
Methodology – Synaesthesia in the Classroom
During
my research of the various types of synaesthesia, I discovered that they each
contained a common thread. Synaesthesia allows the individual to make arbitrary
links between seemingly unrelated visceral stimuli. This ability to
successfully link apparently unrelated ideas and concepts is the first of
Boden’s creative triad.
To
access exploratory and transformative creativity, Boden explains that one must
have a structure. Creativity is “not a matter of abandoning all the rules, but
of changing the existing rules to create a new conceptual space. Constraints on
thinking do not merely constrain, but also make certain thoughts - certain
mental structures - possible” (Boden, 2004: 58). Synaesthesia became the
structure and conceptual space for this particular assignment. In addition to
explaining the neurological phenomenon of synaesthesia, I showed examples of
relevant artwork to my students to demonstrate the creative potential of the synaesthetic
experience. Through my own research, I found evidence of many painters that
exhibited synaesthetic characteristics. For instance, the exhibition catalog
for the show Synaesthesia: Art and the Mind, a show produced by McMaster
Museum of Art, examined the work of legendary synaesthete artists, including Vincent
Van Gogh, Wassily Kandinsky, Charles Burchfield, Joan Mitchell, and Duke
Ellington.Beyond the subject matter, all of the art by the
aforementioned painters includes unusual colour combinations, rhythmic use of
lines and shapes, and parts of the composition enveloped in auras of colour. These
are typical signatures of a synaesthetic artist.
In
my teaching, I was careful to select vivid and impactful works of art, and
describe their compositions. I would ask the students to look at the art
carefully. What other visceral reactions were induced in the scribbles,
scratches, and drips of paint in front of them?
Slowly, they started to make the association between visual imagery and
other sensory reactions. Perhaps that dripping red paint on one of Joan
Mitchell’s canvases was the murky sense of walking through a puddle. Or the
feel of a spring breeze was the yellow aura in a Charles Burchfield landscape. By
explaining the phenomenology of synaestheisa and explaining works of art that
were created by synaesthetes, the students were able to access a new well of
creativity. After my lecture, I encouraged the students to immediately take
this framework of synaesthesia and apply it to the multiple sensory exercise. Based
on the original images they selected, I asked them a series of questions: How does the image make you feel? Do you feel this sensation or
emotion in a particular part of your body? What memories surfaced from
looking at the images? If you could make a soundtrack based on your
image, what would it sound like? What type of media best represents your
image? I stressed that any type of media was
acceptable for the assignment – drawing, painting, collage – as long as it
expressed this cross-modal experience. They now felt that their assignments
were to capture an ineffable, subjective sense of experience. The assignment
has given a structure for exploration and transformation of their minds. The
students continued working on this assignment for another week before a class
critique.
Measurable
Results – How Did The Students Respond? What Types of Work Did They Produce?
After
introducing synaesthesia and various case studies surrounding it, I found that
students’ creativity was intensely heightened. It allowed the students to
access other sensory experiences for inspiration. Along with the artist case
studies and their own sensory perceptions, students started to experiment with
various media to create art. Understanding the cross-modal experience of synaesthesia
gave each student a greater conscious perception of their own experiences of
the world around them.
The
critiques we held in the class meetings following the assignment were always
interesting. Students, often timid to speak in front of groups, could easily
talk for 10 minutes trying to explain the sensation of watching a sunset, and
trying to capture that sensation on paper. Others felt complete and total
freedom in creating nonrepresentational art. They were creating art that
expressed an experience, emotion, or sensory perception and not merely
replicating the likeness of an object. For many, this was the first time they
felt inspired to create this type of art. They felt enabled to experiment with
various media, including: watercolour, gauche, acrylic, markers, and pencils. The
most popular, consistent media used in this assignment was paint.
The
artwork was compelling, and evocative of other sensory perceptions. During the
critiques, the presenters would beam with pride as the audience would shout out
what sense the artwork was communicating. It became a rather fun guessing game,
which furthered into a dialog of subjective and objective experiencing of the
external world. As Boden suggested, introducing synaesthesia as a structure
allowed new thoughts and mental processes to develop in the students’ minds.
How
Teaching this Methodology has Impacted my Own Creative Process
I
have been a painter for years, and noticed an impact on my work after exploring
synaesthesia as a gateway to creativity. First, I moved more toward abstraction.
I started experimenting with various types of paints and carrier oils. Using
unfamiliar media allowed me to let go of the expectation of what I should paint.
Every time I approached my canvas became a big experiment. I learned to allocate time to paint, and
experiment with the media to let the imagery emerge on its own. As a painter,
it is difficult to express to another person that moment when you know the work
is finished. However, I started to realize when my paintings where finished
when they evoked a certain sensory perception or memory.[1]
For
example, the image below is a detail of a recent painting I made. It is a
cross-modal representation of raindrops dripping on a puddle. The dark circles
are the visual phenomenon of the rain falling on the puddle, while the silver
paint is the soft sound of the water making contact with the ground. The
painting thus merges the visual field with the aural and perceptual fields of
experience.
Through
my own artistic practice and my teaching, I have shown that understanding the
phenomenology of synaesthesia can lead to a heightened sense of creativity. It
expands the awareness of the individual’s sensory perception of the world and
it becomes easier to experience the external world holistically. Synaesthesia
illustrates a new structure for making arbitrary links between unrelated
visceral stimuli. It also allows the non-synaesthetic artist to abandon the
traditional narrow scope of what art should be. Creating art thus becomes a
spontaneous interpretation of the senses to illustrate the subjective
experience of life.
References
Boden,
Margaret A. (2004) The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. (New York:
Routledge)
De
Young, R. (2014). Using the Stroop effect to test our capacity to direct
attention: A tool for navigating urgent transitions. Retrieved from http://www.snre.umich.edu/eplab/demos/st0/stroopdesc.html
Ramachandran,
V.S. and Edward M. Hubbard (2003) 'Hearing Colours, Tasting Shapes,' The Scientific American, May 2003.
[1] For more on my personal reflections of creating art, please visit: http://monicadmurgia.com/category/ineffable/